版權(quán)說明:本文檔由用戶提供并上傳,收益歸屬內(nèi)容提供方,若內(nèi)容存在侵權(quán),請進(jìn)行舉報或認(rèn)領(lǐng)
文檔簡介
1、2019年12月大學(xué)英語六級考試真題(一)2019年12月大學(xué)英語六級考試真題(一)- PAGE 9 -Part IWriting(30 minutes)Directions : For this part, you are allowed 30 minutes to write an essay on the importance of having a sense of social responsibility. You should write at least 150 words but no more than 200 words.Part IIListening Comprehe
2、nsion(25 minutes)Directions: Answer the questions 1 to 4 based on the following conversation.1.A.Magazine reporterB.Fashion designerC.Website designerD.Features editor2.A.Designing sports clothingB.Consulting fashion expertsC.Answering daily emailsD.Interviewing job-seekers3.A.It is challenging.B.It
3、 is fascinating.C.It is tiresome.D.It is fashionable.4.A.Her persistenceB.Her experienceC.Her competenceD.Her confidenceDirections: Answer the questions 5 to 8 based on the following conversation.5.A.It is enjoyable.B.It is educational.C.It is divorced from real life.D.It is adapted from a drama.6.A
4、.All the roles are played by famous actors and actresses. B.It is based on the real-life experiences of some celebrities.C.Its plots and events reveal a lot about Frankies actual life. D.It is written, directed, edite d and produced by Frankie himself.7.A.Go to the theater and enjoy it.B.Recommend i
5、t to her friends.C.Watch it with the man.D.Download and watch it.8.A.It has drawn criticisms from scientists.B.It has been showing for over a decade. C.It is a ridiculous piece of satire.D.It is against common sense.Directions: Answer the questions 9 to 11 based on the following passage.9.A.They are
6、 likely to get injured when moving too fast. B.They believe in team spirit for good performance. C.They need to keep moving to avoid getting hurt.D.They have to learn how to avoid body contact.10.A.They do not have many years to live after retirement. B.They tend to live a longer life with early ret
7、irement. C.They do not start enjoying life until full retirement. D.They keep themselves busy even after retirement.11.A.It prevents us from worrying.B.It slows down our aging process.C.It enables us to accomplish more in life.D.It provides us with more chances to learn.Directions: Answer the questi
8、ons 12 to 15 based on the following passage.12.A.It tends to dwell upon their joyous experiences.B.It wanders for almost half of their waking time. C.It has trouble concentrating after a brain injury.D.It tends to be affected by their negative feelings.13.A.To find how happiness relates to daydreami
9、ng.B.To observe how ones mind affects ones behavior. C.To see why daydreaming impacts what one is doing.D.To study the relation between heal th and daydreaming.14.A.It helps them make good decisions.B.It helps them tap their potentials.C.It contributes to their creativity.D.It contributes to clear t
10、hinking.15.A.Subjects with clear goals in mind outperformed those without clear goals. B.The difference in performance between the two groups was insignificant. C.Non-daydreamers were more focused on their tasks than daydreamers.D.Daydreamers did better than non-daydreamers in task performance.Direc
11、tions: Answer the questions 16 to 18 based on the following recording.16.A.They are the oldest buildings in Europe.B.They are part of the Christian tradition. C.They are renovated to attract tourists.D.They are in worsening condition.17.A.They have a history of 14 centuries.B.They are 40 metres tall
12、 on average.C.They are without foundations.D.They consist of several storeys.18.A.Wood was harmonious with nature.B.Wooden buildings kept the cold out. C.Timber was abundant in Scandinavia.D.The Vikings liked wooden strctures.Directions: Answer the questions 19 to 21 based on the following recording
13、.19.A.Similarities between human babies and baby animals. B.Cognitive features of different newly born mammals. C.Adults influence on children.D.Abilities of human babies.20.A.They can distinguish a happy tune from a sad one.B.They love happy melodies more than sad ones. C.They fall asleep easily wh
14、ile listening to music.D.They are already sensitive to beats and rhythms.21.A.Infantsfacial expressions.B.Babies emotions.C.Babies interaction with adult.D.Infants behaviors.Directions: Answer the questions 22 to 25 based on the following recording.22.A.It may harm the culture of todays workplace.B.
15、It may hinder individual career advancement. C.It may result in unwillingness to take risks.D.It may put too much pressure on team members.23.A.They can hardly give expression to their original views. B.They can become less motivated to do projects of their own. C.They may find it hard to get their
16、contributions recognized. D.They may eventually lose their confidence and creativity.24.A.They can enlarge their professional circle.B.They can get chances to engage in research. C.They can make the best use of their expertise.D.They can complete the project more easily.25.A.It may cause lots of arg
17、uments in a team.B.It may prevent making a timely decision.C.It may give rise to a lot of unnecessary expenses.D.It may deprive a team of business opportunities.Part IIIReading Comprehension(40 minutes)When considering risk factors associated with serious chronic diseases, we often think about healt
18、h indicators such as cholesterol, blood pressure, and body weight. But poor diet and physical inactivity also each increase the risk of heart disease and have a role to play in the development of some cancers. Perhaps worse, the 26effects of an unhealthy diet and insufficient exercise are not limite
19、d to your body, Recent research has also shown that 27in ahigh-fat and high-sugar diet may have negative effects on your brain, causing learning and memory 28.Studies have found obesity is associated with impairments in cognitive functioning, as 29by a range of learning and memory tests, such as the
20、 ability to remember a list of words presented some minutes or hours earlier. There is also a growing body of evidence that diet-induced cognitive impairments can emerge 30within weeks or even days. For example, one study found healthy adults 31to a high-fat diet for five days showed impaired attent
21、ion, memory, and mood compared with a low-fat diet control group. Another study also found eating a high-fat and high- sugar breakfast each day for as little as four days resulted in problems with learning and memory 32to those observed in overweight and obese individuals.Body weight was not hugely
22、different between the groups eating a healthy diet and those on high fat and sugar diets. So this shows negative 33of poor dietary intake can occur even when body weight has not changed 34. Thus, body weight is not always the best indicator of health and a thin person still needs to eat well and exe
23、rcise 35.A.assessedB.assignedC.consequencesD.conspicuouslyE.deficitsF.designatedG.detrimentalH.digestionI.excellingJ.indulgingK.loopholesL.rapidlyM.redundantN.regularlyO.similarIncreased Screen Time and Wellbeing Decline in YouthHave young people never had it so good? Or do they face more challenges
24、 than any previous generation? Our current era in the West is one of high wealth. This means minors enjoy material benefits and legal protections that would have been the envy of those living in the past. But there is an increasing suspicion that all is not well for our youth. And one of the most po
25、pular explanations, among some experts and the popular media, is that excessive screen time is to blame. (This refers to all the attention young people devote to their phones, tablets and laptops.) However, this is a contentious theory and such claims have been treated skeptically by some scholars b
26、ased on their reading of the relevant data.Now a new study has provided another contribution to the debate, uncovering strong evidence that adolescent wellbeing in the United States really is experiencing a decline and arguing that the most likely cause is the electronic riches we have given them. T
27、he background to this is that from the 1960s into the early 2000s, measures of average wellbeing went up in the US. This was especially true for younger people. It reflected the fact that these decades saw a climb in general standards of living and avoidance of mass societal traumas like full-scale
28、war or economic deprivation. However, the screen time hypothesis, advanced by researchers such as Jean Twenge, is that electronic devices and excessive time spent online may have reversed these trends in recent years, causing problems for young peoples psychological health.To investigate, Twenge and
29、 her colleagues dived into the Monitoring the Futuredataset based on annual surveys of American school students from grades 8, 10, and 12 that started in 1991. In total, 1.1 million young people answered various questions related to their wellbeing. Twenges teams analysis of the answers confirmed th
30、e earlier, well-established wellbeing climb, with scores rising across the 1990s, and into the later 2000s. This was found across measures like self-esteem, life satisfaction, happiness and satisfaction with individual domains like job, neighborhood, or friends. But around 2012 these measures starte
31、d to decline. This continued through 2016, the most recent year for which data is available.Twenge and her colleagues wanted to understand why this change in average wellbeing occurred. However, it is very hard to demonstrate causes using non-experimental data such as this. In fact, when Twenge prev
32、iously used this data to suggest a screen time effect, some commentators were quick to raise this problem. They argued that her causal-sounding claims rested on correlational data, and that she had not adequately accounted for other potential causal factors. This time around, Twenge and her team mak
33、e a point of saying that they are not trying to establish causes as such, but that they are assessing the plausibility of potential causes.EFirst, they explain that if a given variable is playing a role in affecting wellbeing, then we should expect any change in that variable to correlate with the o
34、bserved changes in wellbeing. If not, it is not plausible that the variable is a causal factor. So the researchers looked at time spent in a number of activities that could plausibly be driving the wellbeing decline. Less sport, and fewer meetings with peers correlated with lower wellbeing, as did l
35、ess time reading print media(newspapers) and, surprisingly, less time doing homework .(This last finding would appear to contradict another popular hypothesis that it is our burdening of students with assignments that is causing all theproblems.) In addition, more TV watching and more electronic com
36、munication both correlated with lower wellbeing. All these effects held true for measures of happiness, life satisfaction and self-esteem, with the effects stronger in the 8th and 10th-graders.Next, Twenges team dug a little deeper into the data on screen time. They found that adolescents who spent
37、a very small amount of time on digital devicesa couple of hours a weekhad the highest wellbeing. Their wellbeing was even higher than those who never used such devices. However, higher doses of screen time were clearly associated with lower happiness. Those spending 10-19 hours per week on their dev
38、ices were 41 percent more likely to be unhappy than lower-frequency users. Those who used such devices 40 hours a week or more(one in ten teenagers)were twice as likely to be unhappy. The data was slightly complicated by the fact that there was a tendency for kids who were social in the real world t
39、o also use more online communication, but by bracketing out different cases it became clear that the real-world sociality component correlated with greater wellbeing, whereas greater time on screens or online only correlated with poorer wellbeing.So far, so plausible. But the next question is, are t
40、he drops in average wellbeing happening at the same time as trends toward increased electronic device usage? It looks like it-after all, 2012 was the tipping point when more than half of Americans began owning smartphones. Twenge and her colleagues also found that across the key years of 2013-16, we
41、llbeing was indeed lowest in years where adolescents spent more time online, on social media, and reading news online, and when more youth in the United States had smartphones. And in a second analysis, they found that where technology went, dips in wellbeing followed. For instance, years with a lar
42、ger increase in online usage were followed by years with lower wellbeing, rather than the other way around. This does not prove causality, but is consistent with it. Meanwhile, TV use did not show this tracking. TV might make you less happy, but this is not what seems to be driving the recent declin
43、es in young peoples average happiness.A similar but reversed pattern was found for the activities associated with greater wellbeing. For example, years when people spent more time with friends were better years for wellbeing (and followed by better years). Sadly, the data also showed face-to-face so
44、cializing and sports activity had declined over the period covered by the survey. IThere is another explanation that Twenge and her colleagues wanted to address: the impact of the the great recession of 2007-2009, which hit a great number of American families and might be affecting adolescents. The
45、dataset they used did not include economic data, so instead the researchers looked at whether the 2013-16 wellbeing decline was tracking economic indicators. They found some evidence that some crude measures, like income inequality, correlated with changes in wellbeing, but economic measures with a
46、more direct impact, like family income and unemployment rates (which put families into difficulties), had no relationship with wellbeing. The researchers also note that the recession hit some years before we see the beginning of the wellbeing drop, and before the steepest wellbeing decline, which oc
47、curred in 2013.J The researchers conclude that electronic communication was the only adolescent activity that increased at the same time psychological wellbeing declined. I suspect that some experts in the field will be keen to address alternative explanations, such as unassessed variables playing a
48、 role in the wellbeing decline. But the new work does go further than previous research and suggests that screen time should still be considered a potential barrier to young peoples flourishing.The year when most Americans began using smartphones was identified as a turning point in young Americans
49、level of happiness.Scores in various wellbeing measures began to go downward among young Americans in recent years.Unfortunately, activities involving direct contact with people, which contributed to better wellbeing, were found to be on the decline.In response to past critics, Twenge and her co-res
50、earchers stress they are not trying to prove that the use of digital devices reduces young peoples wellbeing.In the last few decades of the 20th century, living standards went up and economic depressions were largely averted in the US.Contrary to popular belief, doing homework might add to students
51、wellbeing.The author believes the researchers new study has gone a step further regarding the impact of screen time on wellbeing.The researchers found that extended screen time makes young people less happy.Data reveals that economic inequality rather than family income might affect peoples wellbein
52、g. 45.Too much screen time is widely believed to be the cause of unhappiness among todays young people.“The dangerous thing about lying is people dont understand how the act changes us.” says Dan Ariely behavioural psychologist at Duke University. Psychologists have documented children lying as earl
53、y as the age of two. Some experts even consider lying a developmental milestone, like crawling and walking, because it requires sophisticated planning, attention and the ability to see a situation from someone elses perspective to manipulate then. But, for most people, lying gets limited as we devel
54、op a sense of morality and the ability to self-regulate.Harvard cognitive neuroscientist Joshua Greene says, for most of us, lying takes work. In studies, he gave subjects a chance to deceive for monetary gain while examining their brains in a functional MRI machine, which maps blood flow to active
55、parts of the brain. Some people told the truth instantly and instinctively. But others opted to lie, and they showed increased activity in their frontal parietal (顱腔壁的) control network, which is involved in difficult or complex thinking. This suggests that they were deciding between truth and dishon
56、esty and ultimately opting for the letter. For a follow-up analysis, he found that people whose neural (神經(jīng)的) reward centers were more active when they won money were also more likely to be among the group of liars suggesting that lying may have to do with the inability to resist temptation.External
57、conditions also matter in terms of when and how often we lie. We are more likely to lie, when we are able to rationalise it, when we are stressed and fatigued or see others being dishonest. And we are less likely to lie when we have moral reminders or when we think others are watching. “We as a soci
58、ety need to understand that, when we dont punish lying, we increase the probability it will happen again.” Ariely says.In a 2016 study published in the journal Nature Neuroscience, Ariely and colleagues showed how dishonesty alters peoples brains, making it easier to tell ties in the future. When pe
59、ople uttered a falsehood, the scientists notice a burst of activity in their amygdala. The amygdala is crucial part of the brain that produces fear, anxiety and emotional responses including that sinking, guilty feeling you get when you lie. But when scientists had their subjects play a game in whic
60、h they won money by deceiving their partner, they noticed the negative signals from the amygdala began to decrease. Not only that, but when people faced no consequences for dishonesty, their falsehood tended to get even more sensational. This means that if you give people multiple opportunities to l
溫馨提示
- 1. 本站所有資源如無特殊說明,都需要本地電腦安裝OFFICE2007和PDF閱讀器。圖紙軟件為CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.壓縮文件請下載最新的WinRAR軟件解壓。
- 2. 本站的文檔不包含任何第三方提供的附件圖紙等,如果需要附件,請聯(lián)系上傳者。文件的所有權(quán)益歸上傳用戶所有。
- 3. 本站RAR壓縮包中若帶圖紙,網(wǎng)頁內(nèi)容里面會有圖紙預(yù)覽,若沒有圖紙預(yù)覽就沒有圖紙。
- 4. 未經(jīng)權(quán)益所有人同意不得將文件中的內(nèi)容挪作商業(yè)或盈利用途。
- 5. 人人文庫網(wǎng)僅提供信息存儲空間,僅對用戶上傳內(nèi)容的表現(xiàn)方式做保護(hù)處理,對用戶上傳分享的文檔內(nèi)容本身不做任何修改或編輯,并不能對任何下載內(nèi)容負(fù)責(zé)。
- 6. 下載文件中如有侵權(quán)或不適當(dāng)內(nèi)容,請與我們聯(lián)系,我們立即糾正。
- 7. 本站不保證下載資源的準(zhǔn)確性、安全性和完整性, 同時也不承擔(dān)用戶因使用這些下載資源對自己和他人造成任何形式的傷害或損失。
最新文檔
- 2024物流金融、信用保險服務(wù)合同
- 2025年度市政道路改造工程設(shè)計與施工總承包合同書3篇
- 2025年IDC機(jī)房租賃合同及網(wǎng)絡(luò)安全評估協(xié)議3篇
- 二零二五版金融租賃合同抵押擔(dān)保與租賃資產(chǎn)處置協(xié)議2篇
- 2025廠房升級改造與設(shè)備更新一體化合同3篇
- 2024跨區(qū)域綠色能源開發(fā)與合作框架合同
- 2025版韻達(dá)快遞業(yè)務(wù)承包及運(yùn)營合同3篇
- 幼兒園2025年度綠化維護(hù)服務(wù)合同2篇
- 二零二五年房車托管與戶外運(yùn)動俱樂部合作合同3篇
- 個人二手手機(jī)買賣合同(2024版)2篇
- 倉庫安全培訓(xùn)考試題及答案
- 中國大百科全書(第二版全32冊)08
- 初中古詩文言文背誦內(nèi)容
- 天然氣分子篩脫水裝置吸附計算書
- 檔案管理項(xiàng)目 投標(biāo)方案(技術(shù)方案)
- 蘇教版六年級上冊100道口算題(全冊完整版)
- 2024年大學(xué)試題(宗教學(xué))-佛教文化筆試考試歷年典型考題及考點(diǎn)含含答案
- 計算機(jī)輔助設(shè)計智慧樹知到期末考試答案章節(jié)答案2024年青島城市學(xué)院
- 知識庫管理規(guī)范大全
- 電腦耗材實(shí)施方案、供貨方案、售后服務(wù)方案
- 環(huán)衛(wèi)項(xiàng)目年終工作總結(jié)
評論
0/150
提交評論